GLORIA

GEOMAR Library Ocean Research Information Access

feed icon rss

Your email was sent successfully. Check your inbox.

An error occurred while sending the email. Please try again.

Proceed reservation?

Export
  • 1
    Online Resource
    Online Resource
    Chicago :University of Chicago Press,
    Keywords: Social behavior in animals. ; Electronic books.
    Type of Medium: Online Resource
    Pages: 1 online resource (442 pages)
    Edition: 1st ed.
    ISBN: 9780226187426
    DDC: 599.5
    Language: English
    Note: Intro -- Contents -- Chapter 1. Culture in the Ocean? -- Chapter 2. Culture? -- Chapter 3. Mammals of the Ocean -- Chapter 4. Song of the Whale -- Chapter 5. What the Dolphins Do -- Chapter 6. Mother Cultures of the Large Toothed Whales -- Chapter 7. How Do They Do It? -- Chapter 8. Is This Evidence for Culture? -- Chapter 9. How the Whales Got Culture -- Chapter 10. Whale Culture and Whale Genes -- Chapter 11. The Implications of Culture: Ecosystems, Individuals, Stupidity, and Conservation -- Chapter 12. The Cultural Whales: How We See Them and How We Treat Them -- This Book Came From and Is Built On . . . -- Notes -- Bibliography -- Index.
    Location Call Number Limitation Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 2
    Online Resource
    Online Resource
    Chicago :University of Chicago Press,
    Keywords: Vertebrates -- Behavior -- Mathematical models. ; Animal societies -- Mathematical models. ; Social behavior in animals -- Mathematical models. ; Electronic books.
    Description / Table of Contents: No detailed description available for "Analyzing Animal Societies".
    Type of Medium: Online Resource
    Pages: 1 online resource (351 pages)
    Edition: 1st ed.
    ISBN: 9780226895246
    DDC: 591.7/82
    Language: English
    Note: Intro -- Contents -- Acknowledgments -- Conventions and Abbreviations -- 1: Analyzing Social Structure -- 1.1: Introduction -- 1.2: What Is Social Structure? -- 1.3: Why Social Structure Is Important -- 1.4: Conceptualizing Animal Societies:A Brief History -- 1.5: Ethology and Behavioral Ecology -- 1.6: Hinde's Ethological Conceptual Framework of Social Structure -- 1.7: Other Definitions and Concepts of Social Structure -- 1.8: Elements and Measures of Social Structure -- 1.9: The Functional Why Questions, and Ecology -- 1.10: Examples of Social Analyses -- 1.11: Problems with Analyzing Social Structure -- 2: Technical Matters -- 2.1: Modes of Scientific Enquiry -- 2.2: Basic Descriptive Statistics -- 2.3: Precision of Statistics: Bootstraps and Jackknives -- 2.4: Hypothesis Testing -- 2.5: Data Matrices -- 2.6: Ordination -- 2.7: Classification -- 2.8: Model Fitting and Selection: the Method of Likelihood and the Akaike Information Criterion -- 2.9: Computer Programs -- 3: Observing Interactions and Associations:Collecting Data -- 3.1: Types of Behavior -- 3.2: Interactions -- 3.3: Associations -- 3.4: Groups -- 3.5: Identifying Individuals -- 3.6: Class Data -- 3.7: Collecting Social Data -- 3.8: Data Formats -- 3.9: Sampling Periods -- 3.10: Attributes of Data Sets -- 3.11: How Large a Data Set Is Needed for Social Analysis? -- box 3.1: Precision and Power of Social Analysis -- 4: Describing Relationships -- 4.1: Relationships -- 4.2: Nonsocial Measures of Relationship -- 4.3: Social Attributes of Individuals, Including Gregariousness -- 4.4: Rates of Interaction -- 4.5: Association Indices -- box 4.1: Choosing an Association Index:Recommendations -- 4.6: Temporal Patterning of Interactions/Associations -- 4.7: Relative Relationships: Multivariate Description of Relationships -- 4.8: Types of Relationships. , 4.9: "Special" Relationships: Permutation Tests for Preferred/Avoided Companionships -- 4.10: Quantifying the Strength of a Relationship and the Bond -- 4.11: Relationships between Classes -- box 4.2: Describing Relationships:Recommendations -- 5: Describing and Modeling Social Structure -- box 5.1: Omitting Individuals from Analyses of Social Structure -- 5.1: Attributes of Social Structure -- 5.2: Single-Measure Displays of Social Structure -- box 5.2: Visual Displays of Social Structure:General Guidelines -- 5.3: Network Analysis -- box 5.3: Network Analyses: Recommendations -- 5.4: Dominance Hierarchies -- box 5.4: Analyzing Dominance Hierarchies: Recommendations -- 5.5: Adding Time: Lagged Association Rates -- box 5.5: Lagged Association Rates:Recommendations and Extensions -- 5.6: Multivariate Methods -- 5.7: Delineating Groups, Units, Communities,and Tiers -- box 5.6: Recommendations for Population Division -- box 5.7: Describing Social Structure: Recommendations -- 6: Comparing Societies -- 6.1: Comparing Social Structures -- 6.2: Classifying Social Structures -- 6.3: How Complex Is My Society? -- 7: What Determines Social Structure, and What Does Social Structure Determine? -- 7.1: The Individual in Society: Roles -- 7.2: The Dyad in Society: Conflict -- 7.3: The Dyad in Society: Cooperation -- box 7.1: Quantitative Methods in the Behavioral Ecology of Social Systems -- 7.4: Environmental Determinants of Social Systems -- 7.5: Mating Systems and Social Systems -- 7.6: Culture in Society -- 8: The Way Forward -- 8.1: Conceptual Frameworks -- 8.2: Subjects of Social Analysis -- 8.3: Collecting Better Data -- 8.4: Improving Analysis -- 9: Appendices -- 9.1: Glossary -- 9.2: A Key Journal and Some Useful Books -- 9.3: Computer Programs -- 9.4: Estimating Social Differentiation -- 9.5: Assessing Unit Size, Group Size, or Community Size. , References -- Index.
    Location Call Number Limitation Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 3
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Oxford, UK : Blackwell Publishing Ltd
    Marine mammal science 18 (2002), S. 0 
    ISSN: 1748-7692
    Source: Blackwell Publishing Journal Backfiles 1879-2005
    Topics: Biology
    Notes: Sounds produced by northern bottlenose whales (Hyperoodon ampullatus) recorded in the Gully, a submarine canyon off Nova Scotia, consisted predominately of clicks. In 428 min of recordings no whistles were heard which could unequivocally be attributed to bottlenose whales. There were two major types of click series, initially distinguished by large differences in received amplitude. Loud clicks (produced by nearby whales socializing at the surface) were rapid, with short and variable interclick intervals (mean 0.07 sec; CV 71%). The frequency spectra of these were variable and often multimodal, with peak frequencies ranging between 2 and 22 kHz (mean 11 kHz, CV 59%). Clicks received at low amplitude (produced by distant whales, presumably foraging at depth) had more consistent interclick intervals (mean 0.40 sec, CV 12.5%), generally unimodal frequency spectra with a mean peak frequency of 24 kHz (CV 7%) and 3 dB bandwidth of 4 kHz. Echolocation interclick intervals may reflect the approximate search distance of an animal, in this case 300 m, comparable to that found for sperm whales. The relationship between click frequency and the size of object being investigated, suggests that 24 kHz would be optimal for an object of approximately 6 cm or more, consistent with the size range of their squid prey.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Location Call Number Limitation Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 4
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Oxford, UK : Blackwell Publishing Ltd
    Marine mammal science 17 (2001), S. 0 
    ISSN: 1748-7692
    Source: Blackwell Publishing Journal Backfiles 1879-2005
    Topics: Biology
    Notes: The use of natural marks in capture-recapture studies can lead to unequal capture probabilities. This paper examined a catalog of northern bottlenose whale (Hyperoodon ampullatus) photographs from the Gully, Nova Scotia, to identify potential sources of heterogeneity. This information can be used to select appropriate individuals and photographs to include in analyses. Individual northern bottlenose whales were sufficiently marked to uniquely identify individuals (x̄= 14.5 marks/individual; range 1-67), but not all mark types persisted over time. Reliable marks were defined as mark types that were not lost over the nine-yeat study period (notches, back indentation, and mottled patches). Individuals were considered reliably marked if they possessed at least one back indentation or mottled patch (located within one dorsal fin width, at the base of the dorsal fin) or a notch on the dorsal fin. Sixty-six percent (SE = 5%) of the population were reliably marked. Longterm analyses (months to years) should use only reliably marked individuals, and the results scaled to account for the rest of the population. Our results also showed that photographic quality affected an observer's ability to identify individuals. For this catalog, quantitative analysis indicated only photographs of Q ≥ 4 (on a 6-point scale with 6 representing the highest quality) should be included in mark-recapture analyses sensitive to heterogeneity.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Location Call Number Limitation Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 5
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Oxford, UK : Blackwell Publishing Ltd
    Marine mammal science 16 (2000), S. 0 
    ISSN: 1748-7692
    Source: Blackwell Publishing Journal Backfiles 1879-2005
    Topics: Biology
    Notes: Fecal samples were collected while following sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) off the Galápagos Islands, Ecuador. They contained 133 upper beaks and 164 lower beaks of cephalopods. Analysis of the lower beaks suggests that the sperm whales fed primarily on three genera of cephalopods; Histioteutbis (62%), Ancistrocbeirus (16%), and Octopoteutbis (7%). The beak dimensions indicate that the cephalopods ranged in mantle length from 5 to 54 cm and in mass from 12 to 650 g. Fecal samples varied significantly between five study years and over different parts of the study area, but the number of beaks collected per sample did not correlate significantly with defecation rate (a measure of feeding success). Using beak material from fecal samples gives a biased estimate of sperm whale diet, reducing the frequencies of very small and very large cephalopods. However, all other available methods of assessing sperm whale diet also possess biases.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Location Call Number Limitation Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 6
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Oxford, UK : Blackwell Publishing Ltd
    Marine mammal science 15 (1999), S. 0 
    ISSN: 1748-7692
    Source: Blackwell Publishing Journal Backfiles 1879-2005
    Topics: Biology
    Notes: The behavior of groups of female and immature sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) was measured on 117 d within an 11-yr period off the Galápagos Islands, Ecuador. On each day, up to 18 measures of visually observable behavior were calculated. These concerned speeds, headings, movement patterns, diving synchrony, foraging formations, time spent socializing, and aerial behavior. The measured behavior of the sperm whales was considerably more variable when they were socializing than when foraging. None of the measures showed much correlation with sea-surface temperature, and only measures of consistency of movement were significantly correlated with defecation rate, an indicator of feeding success. However, month-long time periods accounted for over 50% of the variance in eight of eighteen measures, and, in the cases of surface speed and dive synchrony, the effects were statistically significant. In contrast, there was no autocorrelation with lag of one day in the residuals of any of the measures. Thus, behavior may be tracking substantial temporal variation in the whales' environment over scales of about several months. Groups of whales had significantly different travel patterns, but there was little other evidence for group-specific behavior, perhaps because tests of group-specific effects were not of adequate statistical power. Variation in sperm whale behavior, especially over time scales of a few months or longer and spatial scales of a few hundred kilometers or larger, should be considered when estimating densities from sighting surveys.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Location Call Number Limitation Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 7
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Oxford, UK : Blackwell Publishing Ltd
    Marine mammal science 15 (1999), S. 0 
    ISSN: 1748-7692
    Source: Blackwell Publishing Journal Backfiles 1879-2005
    Topics: Biology
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Location Call Number Limitation Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 8
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Oxford, UK : Blackwell Publishing Ltd
    Marine mammal science 17 (2001), S. 0 
    ISSN: 1748-7692
    Source: Blackwell Publishing Journal Backfiles 1879-2005
    Topics: Biology
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Location Call Number Limitation Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 9
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Oxford, UK : Blackwell Publishing Ltd
    Marine mammal science 13 (1997), S. 0 
    ISSN: 1748-7692
    Source: Blackwell Publishing Journal Backfiles 1879-2005
    Topics: Biology
    Notes: The distribution and behavior of mature (12.3–16.3 m) male sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) were studied on the Galápagos Islands breeding ground from April to June 1995. In contrast to previous research seasons when males were observed only in close spatial and temporal proximity to mixed schools of females and immature animals, in 1995 males were sighted in loose aggregations, separated by hours to days from our vessel's encounters with mixed schools. Only one of ten identified males was observed in spatial proximity to a mixed school.Aggregations consisted of two to four (minimum estimates) mature males travelling within a range of a few kilometers and were characterized by consistency of heading among individuals. Aggregations moved over time. During encounters, one to three males were observed at the surface at the same time, with interindividual distances of less than 1,000 m. Synchrony of heading was apparent between spatial associates, and its extent appeared to be related to interindividual distance. Clustering (two or more individuals within 100 m) was observed on only two occasions. No agonistic behaviors were seen.Functions of mature male aggregation on a breeding ground remain unclear. Possible explanations for our observations are local prey abundance, or some form of sociality mediated over spatial scales of hundreds to thousands of meters.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Location Call Number Limitation Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 10
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Oxford, UK : Blackwell Publishing Ltd
    Marine mammal science 11 (1995), S. 0 
    ISSN: 1748-7692
    Source: Blackwell Publishing Journal Backfiles 1879-2005
    Topics: Biology
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Location Call Number Limitation Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
Close ⊗
This website uses cookies and the analysis tool Matomo. More information can be found here...