GLORIA

GEOMAR Library Ocean Research Information Access

feed icon rss

Your email was sent successfully. Check your inbox.

An error occurred while sending the email. Please try again.

Proceed reservation?

Export
  • 1
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Springer
    Plant ecology 85 (1989), S. 1-11 
    ISSN: 1573-5052
    Keywords: Foliage-height profile ; LAI ; Leaf community ; Litter phenology ; Stratification ; Vertical structure
    Source: Springer Online Journal Archives 1860-2000
    Topics: Biology
    Notes: Abstract A combination of optical measurements of leaf heights and observations on litterfall provided a vertical and temporal description of the leaf community structure in a tall, Liriodendron forest on the Maryland coastal plain. Leaf area, mass, and number were bimodally distributed with height. Median leaf number occurs far below (7–8 m) and median leaf mass far above (22–23 m) the median leaf area (18–19 m). Tree species exhibited leaf stratification into 3 height levels: understory (0–10 m), mid canopy (10–25 m), and overstory (25–37 m). Species leaf area in litterfall was related to the species basal area, although representation of leaf number in litterfall was not correlated with stem numbers for species in the stand. Species also showed a clear phenological sequence of leaf fall.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Location Call Number Limitation Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 2
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Springer
    Plant ecology 80 (1989), S. 167-181 
    ISSN: 1573-5052
    Keywords: DCA ; Indicator analysis ; Ordination ; TWINSPAN
    Source: Springer Online Journal Archives 1860-2000
    Topics: Biology
    Notes: Abstract Data from 300 forest stands, scattered over 29 states within the eastern North American deciduous forest, were subjected to detrended correspondence analysis (DCA) and two-way indicator species analysis (TWINSPAN) in an effort to identify classifiable units. Most species are widespread which provide a great deal of continuity in the vegetation. The deciduous forest can be divided into three forest regions: (1) northern, (2) central and (3) southern. The northern region corresponds to the hemlock-white pine-northern hardwood forest of Braun (1950). The central region includes the beech-maple and oak-hickory forests. The beech-maple as identified here includes the mixed mesophytic, beech-maple, maple-basswood and about half of the western mesophytic forests of Braun (1950). The oak-hickory includes Braun's oak-hickory, oak-chestnut and about half of the western mesophytic forests. The southern region coincides with the southern mixed hardwood forests.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Location Call Number Limitation Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
  • 3
    Electronic Resource
    Electronic Resource
    Springer
    Journal of bioeconomics 1 (1999), S. 191-203 
    ISSN: 1573-6989
    Keywords: insect mating ; scaling rules ; sexual selection ; sperm competition
    Source: Springer Online Journal Archives 1860-2000
    Topics: Biology , Economics
    Notes: Abstract In dung flies, copula duration $$(\bar t)$$ decreases, and the proportional rate of sperm transfer $$(\bar c)$$ increases, as male body size becomes larger. From a marginal value approach to optimal copula duration, we show that these relationships result in the product, $$\bar c \cdot \bar t$$ , remaining approximately constant across the range of male body size. The expected proportion of a female's eggs fertilized by a copulating male, equivalent to 〈1 − e−c·t 〉, is likewise invariant with male body size. (Overbars and 〈 〉 refer to the averages over female sizes). We assume that the information or cues a male can perceive about females forms a set of discrete “recognition categories”, each of which is uniquely recognizable by a male, but within which he cannot discriminate. There are then likely to be different male optima for the product $$\bar c \cdot \bar t$$ between categories. But the invariance rules still hold within categories, independently of exactly what the recognition categories are, provided that all males perceive the same categories. For example, suppose that males of all sizes categorise females as either 'large', 'medium', or 'small'. Then though the optimal male strategy (product $$\bar c \cdot \bar t$$ ) for (say) 'large' females may differ from the corresponding optima for the other two categories, it remains constant with male size across all the 'large' females. Further, the product $$\bar c \cdot \bar t$$ should remain constant for all male sizes if we take the average across all females, or across any subset of recognition categories. We believe that these conclusions have general applicability and implications for optimal foraging under the marginal value theorem, and demonstrates how we can sometimes make predictions (e.g. the relation between copula duration and male body size in dungflies) without determining exactly what a forager 'knows'.
    Type of Medium: Electronic Resource
    Location Call Number Limitation Availability
    BibTip Others were also interested in ...
Close ⊗
This website uses cookies and the analysis tool Matomo. More information can be found here...