Skip to main content
Log in

Behavioral responses to echolocation calls from sympatric heterospecific bats: implications for interspecific competition

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Mutual recognition is the product of species coexistence, and has direct effects on survival and reproduction of animals. Bats are able to discriminate between sympatric different heterospecifics based on their echolocation calls, which has been shown both in free-flying and captive bats. To date, however, the factors that may determine the behavioral responses of bats to echolocation calls from sympatric heterospecifics have rarely been tested, especially under well-controlled conditions in captive bats. Hence, we aimed at tackling this question by performing playback experiments (habituation–dishabituation) with three horseshoe bat species within the constant-frequency bat guild, which included big-eared horseshoe bats (Rhinolophus macrotis), Blyth’s horseshoe bats (Rhinolophus lepidus), and Chinese horseshoe bats (Rhinolophus sinicus). We studied the behavioral responses of these three species to echolocation calls of conspecifics, to other two species, and to another heterospecifics bat, Stoliczka’s trident bat (Asellisus stoliczkanus), which also belongs to this guild. We found that the three rhinolophid species displayed a series of distinct behaviors to heterospecific echolocation but few to conspecific calls after habituation, suggesting that they may have been able to discriminate sympatric heterospecific echolocation calls from those of conspecifics. Interestingly, the behavioral responses to heterospecific calls were positively correlated with the interspecific overlap index in trophic niche, whereas call design had only a minor effect. This implies that the behavioral responses of these bats to heterospecific echolocation calls may be related to the degree of interspecific food competition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abrams P (1980) Some comments on measuring niche overlap. Ecology 61:44–49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arlettaz R, Perrin N, Hausser J (1997) Trophic resource partitioning and competition between the two sibling bat species Myotis myotis and Myotis blythii. J Anim Ecol 66:897–911

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold SJ, Bennett AF (1988) Behavioural variation in natural populations. V. Morphological correlates of locomotion in the garter snake (Thamnophis radix). Biol J Linn Soc 34:175–190

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balcombe JP, Fenton MB (1988) Eavesdropping by bats: the influence of echolocation call design and foraging strategy. Ethology 79:158–166

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barclay RMR (1982) Interindividual use of echolocation calls: eavesdropping by bats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 10:271–275

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bee MA (2006) Individual recognition in animal species. In: Naguib M (ed) The encyclopedia of language and linguistics, , 2nd edn. Elsevier, London, pp 617–626

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunet-Rossinni AK, Wilkinson GS (2009) Methods for age estimation and the study of senescence in bats. In: Kunz TH, Parsons S (eds) Ecological and behavioral methods for the study of bats. The John Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, pp 315–325

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell P, Schneider CJ, Zubaid A, Adnan AM, Kunz TH (2007) Morphological and ecological correlates of coexistence in Malaysian fruit bats (Chiroptera: Pteropodidae). J Mammal 88:105–118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caspers B, Wibbelt G, Voigt CC (2009) Histological examinations of facial glands in Saccopteryx bilineata (Chiroptera, Emballonuridae), and their potential use in territorial marking. Zoomorphology 128:37–43

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cody ML (1969) Convergent characteristics in sympatric bird populations: a possible relation to interspecific territoriality. Condor 71:223–234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cody ML (1973) Character convergence. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 4:189–211

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coolen I, Bergen YV, Day RL, Laland KN (2003) Species difference in adaptive use of public information in sticklebacks. Proc R Soc Lond B 270:2413–2419

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dorado-Correa AM, Goerlitz HR, Siemers BM (2013) Interspecific acoustic recognition in two European bat communities. Front Physiol 4:192

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fenton MB (2003) Eavesdropping on the echolocation and social calls of bats. Mammal Rev 33:193–204

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Findley JS (1993) Bats: a community perspective. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Forsman JT, Mönkkönen M, Helle P, Inkeröinen J (1998) Heterospecific attraction and food resources in migrants’ breeding patch selection in northern boreal forests. Oecologia 115:278–286

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillam EH (2007) Eavesdropping by bats on the feeding buzzes of conspecifics. Can J Zool 85:795–801

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale E, Beauchamp G, Magrath RD, Nieh JC, Ruxton GD (2010) Interspecific information transfer influences animal community structure. Trends Ecol Evol 25:354–361

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grether GF, Losin N, Anderson CN, Okamoto K (2009) The role of interspecific interference competition in character displacement and the evolution of competitor recognition. Biol Rev 84:617–635

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin DR (1958) Listening in the dark. Yale University Press, New Haven

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffin DR, Webster FA, Michael CR (1960) The echolocation of flying insects by bats. Anim Behav 8:141–154

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heller KG, von Helversen O (1989) Resource partitioning of sonar frequency bands in rhinolophoid bats. Oecologia 80:178–186

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurd CR (1996) Interspecific attraction to the mobbing calls of black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 38:287–292

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs DS, Barclay RMR (2009) Niche differentiation in two sympatric sibling bat species Scotophilus dinganii and Scotophilus mhlanganii. J Mammal 90:879–887

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs DS, Barclay RMR, Walker MH (2007) The allometry of echolocation call frequencies of insectivorous bats: why do some species deviate from the pattern? Oecologia 152:583–594

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jiang TL, Lu GJ, Sun KP, Luo JH, Feng J (2013) Coexistence of Rhinolophus affinis and Rhinolophus pearsoni revisited. Acta Theriol 58:47–53

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone RA (2001) Eavesdropping and animal conflict. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98:9177–9180

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones G (1996) Does echolocation constrain the evolution of body size in bats? Symp Zool Soc Lond 69:111–128

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones G, Siemers BM (2011) The communicative potential of bat echolocation pulses. J Comp Physiol A 197:447–457

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerth G (2008) Causes and consequences of sociality in bats. Bioscience 58:737–746

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korner P, Whiting MJ, Willem J, Ferguson H (2000) Interspecific aggression in flat lizards suggests poor species recognition. Afr J Herpetol 49:139–146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunz TH, Whitaker JO (1983) An evaluation of fecal analysis for determining food habits of insectivorous bats. Can J Zool 61:1317–1321

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch A, Baker AJ (1991) Increased vocal discrimination by learning in sympatry in 2 species of chaffinches. Behaviour 116:109–126

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marler P (1957) Specific distinctiveness in the communication signals of birds. Behaviour 11:13–39

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore RF (1978) Interspecific aggression: toward whom should a mockingbird be aggressive? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 3:173–176

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parsons S, Jones G (2000) Acoustic identification of twelve species of echolocating bat by discriminant function analysis and artificial neural networks. J Exp Biol 203:2641–2656

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Qvarnström A, Haavie J, Sæther SA, Eriksson D, Pärt T (2006) Song similarity predicts hybridization in flycatchers. J Evol Biol 19:1202–1209

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Racey PA (2009) Reproductive assessment of bats. In: Kunz TH, Parsons S (eds) Ecological and behavioral methods for the study of bats. The John Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, pp 249–264

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliffe JM, ter Hofstede HM (2005) Roosts as information centres: social learning of food preferences in bats. Biol Lett 1:72–74

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russo D, Jones G (2002) Identification of twenty-two bat species (Mammalia: Chiroptera) from Italy by analysis of time-expanded recordings of echolocation calls. J Zool 258:91–103

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ (1983) Frequency modulated calls and species recognition in a Neotropical frog. J Comp Physiol 150:217–221

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Safi K, Kerth G (2007) Comparative analyses suggest that information transfer promoted sociality in male bats in the temperate zone. Am Nat 170:465–472

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schnitzler H-U, Kalko EKV (2001) Echolocation by insect-eating bats. Bioscience 51:557–569

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoeman MC, Jacobs DS (2011) The relative influence of competition and prey defences on the trophic structure of animalivorous bat ensembles. Oecologia 166:493–506

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoener TW (1968) The Anolis lizards of Bimini: resource partitioning in a complex fauna. Ecology 49:704–726

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoener TW (1970) Nonsynchronous overlap of lizards in patchy habitats. Ecology 51:408–418

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schuchmann M, Siemers BM (2010) Behavioral evidence for community-wide species discrimination from echolocation calls in bats. Am Nat 176:72–82

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siemers BM, Swift SM (2006) Differences in sensory ecology contribute to resource partitioning in the bat Myotis bechsteinii and Myotis nattereri (Chiroptera Vespertilionidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 59:373–380

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singaravelan N, Marimuthu G (2008) In situ feeding tactics of short-nosed fruit bat (Cynopterus sphinx) on mango fruits: evidence of extractive foraging in a flying mammal. J Ethol 26:1–7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stefanski RA, Falls JB (1972) A study of distress calls of song, swamp and white-throated sparrows (Aves: Fringillidae). II. Interspecific responses and properties used in recognition. Can J Zool 50:1513–1525

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Übernickel K, Tschapka M, Kalko EKV (2013) Selective eavesdropping behaviour in three Neotropical bat species. Ethology 119:66–76

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voigt-Heucke SL, Taborsky M, Dechmann DKN (2010) A dual function of echolocation: bats use echolocation calls to identify familiar and unfamiliar individuals. Anim Behav 80:59–67

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitaker JO (1988) Food habits of insectivorous bats. In: Kunz TH (ed) Ecological and behavioral methods for the study of bats. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, pp 171–189

    Google Scholar 

  • Yovel Y, Melcon ML, Franz MO, Denzinger A, Schnitzler H-U (2009) The voice of bats: how greater mouse-eared bats recognize individuals based on their echolocation calls. PLoS Comput Biol 5:e1000400

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The work was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant no. 31030011, 31270414, 31100280, 31100281). We would like to thank Mr. Fushun Yang and his family, Lifen Xie, and Xiong Guo for their help in the field, and Jinhong Luo for his invaluable suggestions and comments on the manuscript. We also thank Songtao Yang for his help in making the experimental box and Lei Wang for his help in diet analysis of bats.

Ethical standards

All experiments were approved by the Department of Wildlife Administration in Shuanghe Township, Yunnan province, China.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Walter Metzner or Jiang Feng.

Additional information

Communicated by C. Voigt

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Li, Y., Wang, J., Metzner, W. et al. Behavioral responses to echolocation calls from sympatric heterospecific bats: implications for interspecific competition. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 68, 657–667 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-013-1680-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-013-1680-9

Keywords

Navigation